Mid vowel alternations
Plan for today
- Mid vowel alternations and why they matter
- Mid vowels alternations in Russian: phonology or morphology?
- Extending the analysis: Polish, Ukrainian, Bulgarian
- Morphology vs. the standard generative approach
The basic pattern
Common Slavic vowels
Height | Front | Central | Back |
---|---|---|---|
High | i | y | u |
High reduced | ь | ъ | |
Mid | ě e ę | o ǫ | |
Low | a |
- The *ě vowel (yat’, Cyrillic ѣ) has varying reflexes across the Slavic world (e.g. Samilov 1964)
- Mid or high vowel or diphthong (Russian dialects, Ukrainian, parts of BCMS…)
- Low vowel (Eastern Bulgarian…)
- Merger with mid vowel (Polish, Russian…)
Relevant processes
- Mid vowel backing *e > o (Polish, Sorbian, East Slavic)
- Mid vowel lowering *e > a (Polish, Sorbian)
- Yat’ lowering *ě > a (Polish)
- Yat’ raising *ä > e (Czech, Bulgarian)
The details differ, but two constants remain:
- The preceding vowel stays soft even if the following vowel is back
- Front vowels occur if there is something front in the right-hand context
Mid vowel alternations
The common denominator is that synchronically these processes tend to yield a pattern of alternation where
- X ~ X
- Y ~ Y
- X ~ Y
all exist.
Non-alternating e | Non-alternating ’a | ’a ~ e |
---|---|---|
čest ~ česti ‘frequent’ | pol’ana ~ pol’ani ‘clearing’ | b’al ~ beli ‘white’ |
oves ~ ovesen ‘oat(y)’ | jarъk ~ jarki ‘bright’ | r’adъk ~ redki ‘rare’ |
med ~ meden ‘honey(ed)’ | kn’az ~ kn’azi ‘prince’ | sn’ag ~ snežen ‘snow(y)’ |
elen ~ eleni ‘deer’ | xil’ada ~ xil’adi ‘thousand’ | c’al ~ celi ‘whole’ |
led ~ leden ‘ice’ ~ ‘icy’ | kaf’av ~ kaf’avi ‘brown’ | gol’am ~ golemi ‘big’ |
Normally, we can handle this if X ~ Y happens in some kind of conditioned environment. This is not the case here: the distribution is basically random
- Non-alternating e < *e, *ę, also *ě where the raising context is present across the board
- Non-alternating ’a < *ja, borrowings
- Alternation < *ě in non-raising ~ raising contexts
Why do mid vowel alternations matter?
- The three-way pattern has been taken as evidence for highly abstract analyses
- Sequences like [Cʲo Cʲa] are problematic if palatalization always comes from a following front vowel, and need an account
- The alternations are riddled with exceptions and morphological conditions
Mid vowel alternations in Russian
The basic pattern
In Russian (and Belarusian), *e > o after a soft consonant (including č ž) before a hard consonant, and *ě > e across the board
- *e > o also happens word-finally
- The effect is only reliably visible in stressed syllables because of unstressed vowel reduction: /e/ and /o/ are not distinct after Cʲ
- The alternation is restricted to the context after a soft consonant, with the exception of the historically soft *š ž
Stable e | Stable o | e ~ ’o alternation |
---|---|---|
strélɨ ~ strél’bɨ ‘shoot’ | t’ótuška ~ t’ót’a ‘aunt’ | jel’ ~ jólka ‘fir tree’ |
b’élɨj ~ b’el’en’kij ‘white’ | ved’óm ~ ved’ót’e ‘PRS theme vowel’ | sél’sk’ij ~ s’ola ‘village’ |
v’éra ~ v’ér’it’ ‘believe’ | jož ~ jóžɨt’s’a ‘hedgehog’ | žónɨ ~ žén’it ‘wife’ |
That looks like a lot of exceptions!
Some concerns
- Non-alternating o should lead us to expect underlying /o/, but then why are the preceding consonants soft?
- ‘Before a soft consonant’ does not quite work, because the immediately following consonant can be (or at least seem) hard in a cluster:
- č’ért’i ‘devil.PL’ ~ č’órt ‘devil.SG’
- s’éstr’in ‘sisterly’ ~ s’óstrɨ ‘sisters’
That said, in a lot of these cases soft and hard consonants do not contrast in clusters: there is a phonological contrast between [str] and [str’] in Russian, but not between [str] and [st’r]. Assiming the softness of the final consonant is derived from the suffix, as we should, we end up with an apparent Duke-of-York derivation /str+i/ → /s’t’r’/ → [str’] (Phonetically, in fact, these consonants are usually ‘intermediate’, i.e. neither palatalized like soft ones nor velarized like hard ones. At least that’s the claim in the literature!)
- We regularly find ’o before consonants softened by an epenthetic vowel: m’ótl-ɨ ‘brooms’, v’ódr-a ‘buckets’ ~ GEN.PL m’ót’el, v’ód’er
Mid vowel alternations and morphology
Regular form | Unexpected form | Suffix |
---|---|---|
m’od ‘honey’ | m’ód’e | LOC.SG |
m’órznut ‘freeze’ | m’órzl’i | PST.PL |
m’órzn’i | IMP.2SG | |
p’os ‘dog’ | p’ós’ik | DIM |
The classical analysis
What would Lightner (1965) do?
That’s right! An underlying /ѣ/! (Lightner 1969)
Rule | /vēr-ō/ | /vēr-ī-tī/ | /sĕl-ō/ | /sĕl-ĭsk-/ |
---|---|---|---|---|
Palatalization | vʲērō | vʲērʲītʲī | sʲĕlō | sʲĕlʲĭsk- |
ĕ \(\rightarrow\) ŏ / _C | sʲŏlō | |||
Vowel shifts | vʲera | vʲerʲitʲ | sʲola | sʲelʲsk- |
What about morphology?
The initial response is cyclicity
Cycle | Rule | (mĕd)ŭ | (mĕd)ē |
---|---|---|---|
First cycle | Palatalization | mʲĕd | mʲĕd |
Vowel backing | mʲŏd | mʲŏd | |
Second cycle | Palatalization | mʲŏd+ŭ | mʲŏdʲ+ē |
Vowel shifts | mʲod | mʲodʲe |
More problematic is vacillation in apparently identical morphological environments:
Basic form | Gloss | Derived form | Gloss |
---|---|---|---|
kol’ós-a | wheel-PL | kol’és-nik | ‘wheelwright’ |
t’en’ót-a | mesh-PL | t’en’ót-nik | ‘spider’ |
- The implied cyclic structure is (kŏlĕs-ĭn-īk)ŭ but ((tĕnĕt)-ĭn-īk)ŭ — but why?
A solution: consonant power
Hamilton (1976):
- Vowel power: soft consonants are derived from following front vowels, not phonemic1 (Lightner 1963; Lightner 1965)
- Consonant power: traditional approach with phonemic soft consonants, in evidence from the start and through at least Halle (1959)
1 That is, not found in URs
Rule | /ver-a/ | /veriti/ | /sʲola/ | /sʲol-ьsk-/ |
---|---|---|---|---|
Palatalization | vʲera | vʲerʲiti | sʲola | sʲolʲьsk- |
o \(\rightarrow\) e / Cʲ_Cʲ | sʲelʲьsk- | |||
Yer fall | vʲera | vʲerʲitʲ | sʲola | sʲelʲsk- |
- Consonant softness is a combination of
- Derived before front vowels: to account for palatalizing suffixes
- Underlying, visible before back vowels
Hamilton (1976) and Polivanova (1976) are (sort of) able to pull this off, because the structure of the Russian mid vowel alternation is somewhat different from that of the Bulgarian (Table 1):
- In Bulgarian, both [e] ~ [e] (underlying /e/?) and [a] ~ [a] (underlying /a/?) patterns occur after Cʲ, so [e] ~ [a] can’t be either /e/ or /a/
- In Russian, [e] ~ [e] and [e] ~ [o] occur after Cʲ, but [o] ~ [o] is — overwhelmingly — restricted to preceding C. This opens up the possibility that [e] ~ [o] is /Cʲo/ with a fronting rule
Unfortunately the prediction is that surface [CʲoCʲ] should be impossible other than by cyclicity, and this is wrong:
- Exceptions in suffixes: n’es’-ó-t’e ‘carry-PRS-2PL’, z’eml’-ój ‘earth-INS.SG’…
- Non-alternations in root morphemes: t’ót’-a ‘aunt’, šč’óč-k-a ‘cheek-DIM’, p’ós’-ij ‘canine’…
- Non-alternations before Cʲ followed by yer: v’ód’er ‘bucket.GEN.PL’ (cf. v’ódr-a ‘bucket.NOM.PL’)
We need to either write them off, which seems suboptimal, or posit different URs for alternating and non-alternating /o/, which puts us back to square one, or even further behind (Itkin 2007:235–236)
The real solution: morphology
Three components of a working solution
- Face-value underliers: [e] ~ [e] = /e/, [o] ~ [o] = /o/
- Consonant Power is right in not deriving [Cʲo] from /Ce/, resolving many exceptions
- The right-hand context is not the softness of the consonant but whether the suffix triggers softening (Itkin 1994; Itkin 2007; Cubberley 2002)
- Consonant clusters are not an issue
- Soft consonants before a yer are not an issue: the GEN.PL zero/yer suffix is not softening
Here we encounter a key difference between structuralist morphophonemic approaches and generative phonology. In structuralism, as well as ‘poststructuralism’ as practised at least in Russia, morphophonology retains in autonomous status: the units are purely abstract and do not have phonological content. By contrast, in generative phonology the currency is always phonological units with phonological distinctive features. This is a big part of the reason the vowel power approach is so attractive: ‘softening’ suffixes soften not by accident but because soft consonants and front vowels are both [\(-\)back], and here we find that a front vowel is selected by softening suffixes. A more representationally elaborate generative approach should be able to keep the link between the phonological side of the alternation and its triggering intact.
- Focus on morphology over phonology mean we are in a better position to understand the morphosyntactic entanglements in play
Some morphological findings
Itkin (2007) identifies several ‘indifferent’ suffixes, which soften preceding consonants but do not front an alternating vowel. It turns out that this class is not trivial morphologically or semantically. See Iosad (2020) for details.
- Fronting inflectional suffixes do not trigger front vowels (Table 2)
- Some of the most productive and semantically trivial suffixes do not trigger front vowels
- Diminutive -ik (p’ós’-ik ‘dog-DIM’, č’órt’–ik ‘devil-DIM’)
- Diminutive -en’k (t’ópl’-en’k’-ij ‘warm-DIM’, p’óstr’-en’k’-ij ‘multicoloured-DIM’)
- Diminutive -ec (šč’ót’-ec ‘bill-DIM’, v’ed’ór-c-e ‘bucket.DIM’)
- Productive, semantically trivial, creating phonological opacity = Class 2, word-level
- Semantically nontrivial, phonologically transparent = Class 1, stem-level
Suffixes that force fronting of non-alternating vowels tend to look more like ‘Class 1’ suffixes in languages like English (-al, -ity): they are not always very productive, tend to create non-idiomatic semantics, but — as we see with the mid vowel alternations — are able to influence the phonology of what they attach to. By contrast, the ‘indifferent’ suffixes are either inflectional or highly productive and semantically trivial, and they do not influence the phonology, leading to opaque effects. This is a lot like English ‘Class 2’.
Summing up
- The mid vowel alternation in Russian behaves a lot like stem-level phonology in other languages.2 In one way, this is not surprising: the complicated derivations posited for Slavic have long been interpreted along stratal lines (Blumenfeld 2003; Rubach 2008). However, the reasons are usually that we need to make the all the rules implied in the traditional generative account work, rather than more explicitly tied to morphonsyntax
- Solutions that rely on morphological structure may not be as circular as argued by their opponents
- Phonological take-away: less need for abstract URs like /ѣ/ if we need to appeal to morphology to explain whether the alternation happens
2 Whatever your theory of that distinction!
The basic mechanics
- Non-alternating [e] is /e/
- Non-alternating [o] is /o/ — including in /Cʲo/
- Alternating [e] ~ [o]:
- Morphology/lexical insertion provides for choice (not rewrite rule!)
- [e] chosen before a softening suffix in the same cycle
Mid vowel alternations in Polish
The inventory of alternations
Common Slavic | Front context | Back context |
---|---|---|
*e | niesi-e ‘carry-PRS.3SG’ | nios-ę ‘carry-PRS.1SG’ |
sklep ‘shop’ | ||
*ě | wierz-e ‘faith-LOC.SG’ | wiar-a ‘faith-NOM.SG’ |
chleb ‘bread.NOM.SG’ | ||
*ь | cześć ‘honour’ | pies ‘dog’ |
At face value, the alternations look quite similar. After a soft consonant, [ɛ] alternates with [ɔ] (if from *e) or with [a] (if from *ě).
- The back context is ‘a following hard coronal’, not just a ‘hard consonant’
- Surface [ɛ] from *ь almost never alternates with a back vowel, but clear examples of non-alternation in a paradigm are hard to come by for independent reasons
- However, cf. dzień ‘day’, GEN.SG dnia, DIM dzionek; wieś ‘village’, GEN.SG wsi, DIM wioska
The abstract solution
By now we should be familiar with how this can be analysed (Lightner 1963; Gussmann 1980)
Rule | /wær-a/ | /wær-ɛ/ | /nɛs-ɛ/ | /nɛs-ɔ̃/ |
---|---|---|---|---|
Palatalization | wʲæra | wʲærʲɛ | nʲɛsʲɛ | nʲɛsɔ̃ |
Pre-coronal backing | wʲara | nʲɔsɔ̃ | ||
Vowel shifts | wʲɛrʲɛ | nʲɔsɛ̃ | ||
Late rules | vʲara | vʲɛʐɛ | ɲɛɕɛ | ɲɔsɛ̃ |
Unlike Russian, taking the back vowel as the UR does not work at all, for at least two reasons:
- Like in Bulgarian, the existence of non-alternating [Cʲa] rules out /Cʲa/ for wiara: polana ‘clearing’ ~ polanie ‘DAT.SG’
- Backing is narrowly conditioned by a hard coronal, front is very clearly the elsewhere context
Some problems with the abstract solution
- Similar issue with consonant clusters: czarny ‘black’ ~ czernić ‘blacken.INF’ (*czerznić), plot-ł-y ‘weave-PST-PL.F’ ~ plet-l-i ‘weave-PST-PL.M’ (*plećli)
- Massive variation and irregularity within and across lexical items affected
NOM.SG.M | NOM.PL.M | CMP | Verb | Gloss |
---|---|---|---|---|
biały | biali | bielszy | bielić | ‘white’ |
blady | bladzi | bladszy ~ bledszy | blednąc | ‘pale’ |
Towards a morphological approach
Unlike Russian, in Polish the mid vowel alternations can occur within inflectional paradigms, as in wiara ~ wierze, niesie ~ niosę. Still, this is rare and visibly on the retreat diachronically.
Gussmann (2007) expressly takes both alternations out of the phonological grammar and treats them as ‘morphological’.
UR | Back context | Front context |
---|---|---|
/bʲal/ | biały ‘white.NOM.SG.M’ | biali ‘white.NOM.SG.F’ |
/bʲɛl/ | bielić ‘white’ | |
/jɛzʲɔr/ | jezioro ‘lake.NOM.SG’ | jeziorze ‘lake.LOC.SG’ |
/jɛzʲɛr/ | pojezierze ‘lake district’ |
That’s fine as far as it goes, but we still need to account for alternations in inflection.
Here, our architecture of suffix-driven allomorph selection seems to offer a way forward. In particular, in the abstract analysis we had to rule out fronting of underlying /Cʲa/ (and perhaps /Cʲo/), because this directionality simply does not work empirically. With allomorph selection, there is no directionality, and we are able to posit a similar analysis to what we did in Russian:
- [ɛ] ~ [ɛ] is underlying /ɛ/
- [Cʲa] ~ [Cʲa] is underlying /Cʲa/
- Alternating [ɛ] ~ [ɔ] and alternating [ɛ] ~ [a] is allomorph selection, with [ɛ] chosen before a softening suffix
This flips the directionality of the alternation, but correctly accounts for the fact that analogical levelling removes front alternants over time
NOM.SG | LOC.SG 18th century | LOC.SG today |
---|---|---|
siostra | siestrze ~ siostrze | siostrze |
jezioro | jezierze ~ jeziorze | jeziorze |
We can formalize the change by saying that levelling removes the conditioned allomorph, leaving the elsewhere allomorph in place.
Extending the framework
Mid vowel alternations in Ukrainian
Ukrainian is unusual within Slavic in that the soft/hard contrast is neutralized before *e i, but the outcome is hard rather than soft. Why this happened and how this works has been a major question for phonologists all the way back to Jakobson’s Remarques.
Ukrainian also has a version of the e ~ o alternation, with the following caveats:
- It only occurs after j č š ž
- It does not occur within inflectional paradigms at all
- There is a lot of levelling across lexical items
Item | Polish | Ukrainian |
---|---|---|
‘evening.GEN.SG’ | wieczoru | večora |
‘evening.LOC.SG’ | wieczorze | večor’i |
‘supper’ | wieczerza | večer’a |
‘black’ | czarny | čornɨj |
‘blacken’ | czernić | čorn’itɨ |
‘monk’ | černec’ |
Morphological structure and cyclicity
- Ukrainian and Polish both show so many exceptions as to make a morphological solution almost inevitable3
- Cyclic effects are stronger in Ukrainian:
- Inflection: Polish (wieczorz)-e, (nies-ie) but Ukrainian only (večor’)-i
- Derivation: Polish (czern-i)-ć vs. Ukrainian ((čorn)-i)-tɨ
3 Not like Russian doesn’t have exceptions! But historically we’ve tended to underplay them
Inflectional suffixes can still play into the alternation in Polish (though marginally by now), but inflectional alternations are fully levelled out in Ukrainian. In denominal and deadjectival verbs, softening stem-forming suffixes can trigger front vowels in Polish, while Ukrainian shows robust opacity.
This comparison should tell us something about the interaction of morphology and phonology, likely in a diachronic perspective.
Back to Bulgarian
- The usual account is this: in items undergoing the [e] ~ [a] alternation4
- [a] occurs before a syllable with a back vowel, unless the intervening consonant (cluster) is or contains j č ž št žd
- [a] occurs in a word-final syllable
- [e] occurs elsewhere, i.e. before a syllable with [i e] or before a postalveolar
- What is the analysis?
4 It is very largely limited to stressed syllables; we ignore this here.
Is it the vowels?
If the alternation is triggered by vowels, how do consonants trigger [e]?
Back context | Gloss | Front context | Gloss |
---|---|---|---|
m’ára | ‘measure’ | mér’ъ | ‘measure.PRS.1SG’ |
s’ánka | ‘shadown’ | zasénčъ | ‘overshadow.PRS.1SG’ |
n’ákoj | ‘someone’ | néshto | ‘something’ |
kr’ásъk | ‘squeak’ | krésl’o | ‘squeaker’ |
Is it the consonants?
Could we say that fronting occurs before a soft consonant?
On Monday we saw that consonants before [i e] are considered phonologically hard, but that does not have to follow: all we know is the contrast is neutralized.
Indeed, the usual account in generative phonology is /Ci/ \(\rightarrow\) [Cʲi]: why can’t this be the case for Bulgarian?
Is it morphology?
Bulgarian also shows a lot of lexical exceptions to the generalizations, due to dialect mixing, inconsistent treatment of bookish borrowings, and general diachronic chaos.
Noun | SG.M | SG.F | PL | Gloss |
---|---|---|---|---|
l’ato | leten | l’atna | letni | ‘summer’ |
v’ara | veren | v’arna | verni | ‘faith’ |
žel’azo | železen | žel’azna | železni | ‘iron’ |
sn’ag | snežen | snežna | snežni | ‘snow’ |
gn’av | gneven | gnevna | gnevni | ‘wrath’ |
cv’at | cveten | cvetna | cvetni | ‘colour’ |
kol’ano | kolenen | kolenna | kolenni | ‘knee’ |
Unlike Russian and Polish (Ukrainian is slightly more complicated), cyclic misapplication in Bulgarian results in more [e]’s than would be expected under the regular pattern, not in overapplcation of backing.
Summary
- Mid vowel alternations used to provide ample support for abstract analyses with absolute neutralization
- In the present-day languages, they tend to be deeply entwined with morphology
- Stratal analyses seem to work well in at least some cases, but much more work remains to be done
- A more surface-oriented analysis that acknowledges the role of morphology is feasible and has some advantages