Palatalization and consonant-vowel interactions
Basic facts and terminology
Basic facts: hard and soft consonants
The distinction between ‘hard’ and ‘soft’ consonants is traditional in both synchronic and diachronic phonology. Very broadly, ‘soft’ consonants are those that are produced by a process that involves front vowels or glides.
Two key processes are secondary palatalization, where a consonant becomes ‘soft’ before a front vowel (that may have disappeared later), and coalescence with a following *j. In principle, any consonant can have a palatalized counterpart, but synchronically we find that we can roughly divide the Slavic languages into two groups:
- Northern (all of East Slavic, Polish, Sorbian) and (Eastern) Bulgarian: basically all consonants1 have soft counterparts, usually realized as palatalized versions of the hard consonant.
- In some languages (Polish, Belarusian, parts of Ukrainian), some soft coronals (especially sonorants) are palatal rather than palatalized anterior
- A common secondary development is enhancement of the soft quality. A typical example is Polish and Belarusian: t’ d’ s’ z’ > [t͡ɕ d͡ʑ ɕ ʑ] and similar
- South Slavic other than (Eastern) Bulgarian, Czech and Slovak: no secondary palatalization contrast, but a distinctive series of palatals: /c ɟ ɲ ʎ/ (and the Czech ř) as the ‘soft’ versions of /t d n l r/
1 With some complications around velars
Hard | Soft | Source | Example |
---|---|---|---|
t d n l | c ɟ ɲ ʎ | *Cj coalescence | Svk žena ‘woman’ ≠ baňa ‘mine’ |
Secondary palatalization | Cz prst ‘finger’ ≠ prsť ‘soil’ | ||
Cz když ‘where’ ≠ divný ‘strange’ | |||
p b t d s z | pʲ bʲ tʲ dʲ sʲ zʲ | Secondary palatalization | Uk pit ‘sweat’ ≠ myt’ ‘moment’ |
n l | nʲ/ɲ lʲ/ʎ | Secondary palatalization | Uk den’ ‘day’ ≠ son ‘dream’ |
*Cj coalescence | Uk kin’ ‘horse’ |
Basic facts: velar palatalizations
A special place belongs to posterior coronals č ž š, and in some languages the affricates c dz. Diachronically, they derive from palatalization processes.
Process | Change | Example | Gloss |
---|---|---|---|
First velar palatalization | k g x > č ž š / _i e ě ь | BCMS muka ~ mučiti | ‘torment’ N ~ INF |
Cj coalescence | sj zj > š ž | Po zwisać ~ wiszę | ‘hang’ INF ~ PRS.1SG |
tj dj > various outcomes | Ru xod ~ xožu | ‘walk’ N ~ PRS.1SG | |
P chód ~ chodzę | |||
BCMS rod ~ rođen | ‘kin, birth’ ~ ‘born’ | ||
Bu rod ~ rozhden | |||
Second velar palatalization | k g x > c dz š/s | BCMS ruka ~ ruci | ‘hand’ NOM ~ DAT |
Since these segments usually derive from ‘C + front vowel’ or *Cj sequences, they share many behaviours with soft consonants. However, with few exceptions2 they are phonetically ‘hard’ (not palatalized or palatal), and usually do not have soft counterparts. They also show some ‘hard’ phonological patterning, as we shall see.
2 Notably c’ <ц> in Ukrainian, [с ɟ] <ќ ѓ> in Macedonian, and ć đ in at least parts of BCMS.
Key alternations and examples
- Surface palatalization
-
C → Cʲ, usually before a front vowel
- (First) velar palatalization
-
k g/ɣ/ɦ x → č ž š, usually before a front vowel or j
- Transitive palatalization
-
t d s z → T D š ž, before a historical j but often without a clear context synchronically
- Labial iotation
-
∅ → lʲ/ʎ after labials, before a historical j but often without a clear context synchronically.
- (Second) velar palatalization
-
k g/ɣ/ɦ x → c dz/z s/š in a very restricted number of contexts
Transitive palatalization and labial iotation often occur in the same contexts as each other. Sometimes — but not always — you also see the first velar palatalization in those contexts, too, but 1VP can also co-occur with Surface Palatalization.
Alternation | Example | Gloss |
---|---|---|
Surface palatalization | P kosa [s] ~ kosić [ɕ] | ‘scythe’ ~ ‘mow.INF’ |
Ru korm ~ kormit’ [mʲ] | ‘feed’ N ~ INF | |
First velar palatalization | Ru muka ~ mučit’ | ‘torment’ N ~ INF |
BCMS jak ~ jači | ‘strong’ POS ~ CMP | |
Transitive palatalization | P kosa [s] ~ koszę [ʂ] | ‘scythe’ ~ ‘mow.PRS.1SG’ |
BCMS ljut ~ ljući [tɕ] | ‘angry’ POS ~ CMP | |
P chód [d] ~ chodzę [d͡z] | ‘walk’ N ~ PRS.1SG | |
Labial iotation | Ru korm ~ korml’u | ‘feed’ N ~ PRS.1SG |
BCMS glup ~ gluplji | ‘stupid’ POS ~ CMP | |
Second velar palatalization | Bu vъlk ~ vъlci | ‘wolf’ SG ~ PL |
P rąka ~ ręce | ‘hand’ NOM ~ DAT |
The problem of /y/ and velars
Many, but not all, present-day languages distiguish between [i] and a second non-back, non-round, high (or near-high) vowel, traditionally symbolized y.3
3 Not IPA [y]!
A detailed overview of the realization and patterning of this vowel can be found in Press (1986).
In most languages that distinguish them in principle, [i] and [ɨ] are in complementary distribution:
- [i] after soft consonants and syllable-initially
- [ɨ] after hard consonants including the unpaired postalveolars č ž c dz
Sequence | Russian | Polish | Ukrainian |
---|---|---|---|
ti di | ✗ | marginal | ✓ |
tʲi dʲi | ✓ | ✓ (basically) | ✓ |
tɨ dɨ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ |
ki ɡi | ✗ | ✗ | ✗ |
kʲ ɡʲi | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ |
kɨ ɡɨ | restricted | restricted | ✓ |
tɛ dɛ | marginal | ✓ | ✓ |
tʲɛ dʲɛ | ✓ | ✓ | restricted |
kɛ ɡɛ | restricted | restricted | ✓ |
kʲɛ ɡʲɛ | ✓ | ✓ | ✗ |
Key:
- ✓ sequence allowed
- ✗ sequence disallowed
- Marginal: allowed but largely restricted to borrowings/newer lexicon
- Restricted: allowed only in certain phonological/morphophonological circumstances
Surface generalizations for Russian and Polish:
- Both [i] and [ɛ] prefer to follow soft consonants
- [ɨ] only follows hard consonants
- General tendency to neutralize [k ɡ] ~ [kʲ ɡʲ] before [i ɨ ɛ] — but to maintain [t d] ~ [tʲ dʲ]
The analysis of /i/ and /y/
- Because of the largely complementary distribution of [i] and [ɨ], they were generally considered to be allophones of /i/ in structuralist phonology
- This is central to Jakobson (1929), is acknowledged by Trubetzkoy (1934), and remains the case in Halle (1959)
- To state the distribution, we have to assume that /C/ and /Cʲ/ are phonemically distinct.
Consonant | Word-final | Before /i/ | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Hard | /pil/ [pɨl] | |||
Soft | /pilʲ/ | [pɨlʲ] | /pʲil/ | [pʲiɫ] |
Consonant palatalization in generative phonology
What needs an account?
- Front vowels generally follow Cʲ
- C and Cʲ can contrast when there is no following front vowel
- There hard → soft alternations before front vowels
- After surface hard consonants, we only find [ɨ]
The generative approach
A large proportion of Slavic morphophonological patterns can be explained if soft consonants always derive from hard consonants followed by a front vowel
As we discussed yesterday, all kinds of alternations are produced by the single mechanism of phonological rule. On this basis, generative phonology can use morphophonological alternations to posit rules and ‘unspool’ them to recover abstract underlying representations.
Two kinds of /i/
Language | Item | Infinitive suffix | Nominative plural |
---|---|---|---|
Russian | xod | xodit’ [dʲi] | xody [dɨ] |
muka | mučit’ | muki [kʲi] | |
Polish | chód | chodzić [d͡ʑi] | chody [dɨ] |
męka | męczyć | męki [kʲi] |
Rule | /xod-itʲ/ | /muk-itʲ/ | /xod-ɨ/ | /muk-ɨ/ |
---|---|---|---|---|
Velar palatalization | mučitʲ | |||
Post-velar fronting | muki | |||
Surface palatalization | xodʲitʲ | mukʲi |
What is going on?
On the face of it, the č in mučit’ cannot derive from a rule turning k into č before [i], because forms like muki show that [kʲi] sequences are allowed. However, such surface sequences occur precisely in contexts where the vowel turns up as [ɨ] after hard consonants when those consonants are not velars. Conversely, the rule that looks like 1VP is triggered by vowels that trigger Surface Palatalization of preceding consonants.
Because of the different morphophonological behaviours, the two suffixes are analysed as containing two different underlying vowels:
- The infinitive has a real front vowel, which triggers assimilation of preceding consonants: Surface Palatalization for non-velars, 1VP for velars
- The nominative plural has a back vowel, which unsurprisingly does not trigger softening — unless a different rule (Post-Velar Fronting) makes it front. If it does end up front, it is able to trigger palatalization like any other front vowel
- This is all phonology, and it works with phonological segments, which have real (phonetically non-trivial) feature specifications and all the rest. The vowels behave this way because they really are front or back, not because of an abstract ‘morphophonemic’ structure
- The fact that this analysis closely tracks diachronic developments is neither surprising nor problematic: this is the system working exactly as intended.
A fun fact: the idea of Post-Velar Fronting has a deep Jakobsonian pedigree. The idea that *ky gy xy developed to *ki gi xi and then the front vowel palatalized the preceding velars is a centrepiece of Jakobson (1929). His explanation remained foundational in structuralist historical accounts, but this history seems to have been largely forgotten in generative analyses; one exception is Padgett (2003).
Getting the complementary distribution
The analysis so far explains the fact that we have [Cʲi] and [Cɨ] but not *[Ci]
- Underlying /Cɨ/ remains
- Underlying /Ci/ → [Cʲi] by Surface Palatalization
There is still a problem with the Polish.
Rule | /xɔd-itʲ/ | /mɛNk-itʲ/ | /xɔd-ɨ/ | /mɛNk-ɨ/ |
---|---|---|---|---|
First Velar Palatalization | mɛNčitʲ | |||
Post-Velar Fronting | mɛNki | |||
Surface Palatalization | xɔdʲitʲ | mɛNčʲitʲ | mɛNkʲi | |
Minor rules | xɔd͡ʑit͡ɕ | mɛnčʲit͡ɕ | mɛŋkʲi | |
Predicted surface form | xɔd͡ʑit͡ɕ | mɛnčʲit͡ɕ | xɔdɨ | mɛŋkʲi |
Actual surface form | mɛnt͡ʂɨt͡ɕ |
This is what I meant when I said that the velar palatalization products behave as soft in the phonology but end up being surface-hard. The cz dż sz ż series in Polish sound hard and also condition a following [ɨ], and we don’t have an account of that yet.
The backness switch
Rule | /xɔd-itʲ/ | /mɛNk-itʲ/ | /xɔd-ɨ/ | /mɛNk-ɨ/ |
---|---|---|---|---|
First Velar Palatalization | mɛNčʲitʲ | |||
Post-Velar Fronting | mɛNki | |||
Surface Palatalization | xɔdʲitʲ | mɛNkʲi | ||
Postalveolar hardening | mɛnčitʲ | |||
Retraction | mɛnčɨtʲ | |||
Minor rules | xɔd͡ʑit͡ɕ | mɛnt͡ʂɨt͡ɕ | xɔdɨ | mɛŋkʲi |
The product of 1VP is soft. When Surface Palatalization stops being relevant,4 a Hardening rule applies and feeds retraction of [i] to [ɨ]
4 The eagle-eyed will notice that Surface Palatalization fails before [ɛ]. Ask me about it if you’re still wondering by the end!
We now correctly derive surface [Cɨ], whether it from underlying /Cɨ/ or from another process where the consonant ends up hard for other reasons, but our derivations are getting really rather long.
Note that underlying /kɨ/, with a back vowel (and a hard consonant), surface with a front vowel and a soft consonant, and underlying /ki/, with a front vowel, surfaces as [t͡ʂɨ], with a hard consonant and a back vowel. The term is due to Rubach (2000)
Further developments
Taking the system further
The basic recipe for analysing palatalization:
- Soft consonants come from following front vowels (or glides)
- If a vowel has a softening effect, it must be underlyingly front
- If a vowel does not have a softening effect, it must be underlyingly back
- Rule ordering will keep us straight
Regressive palatalization revisited
Languages like Czech show coronal → palatal alternations before some, but not all, front vowels
Context | Non-palatalizing | Palatalizing |
---|---|---|
Morpheme-internal | když [di] ‘where’ | divný [ɟi] ‘strange’ |
Adjective inflection | pěkný [ni] ‘beautiful.NOM.SG.M’ | pěkní [ɲi] ‘beautiful.NOM.PL’ |
Nominal inflection | hradem [dɛ] ‘city.INS.SG’ | hradě [ɟɛ] ‘city.LOC.SG’ |
This can be accounted for by positing back underlying vowels with subsequent fronting
Rule | /pjɛkn-ɨː/ | /pjɛkn-iː/ |
---|---|---|
Palatalization | pjɛkɲiː | |
Vowel fronting | pjɛkniː |
In general colloquial Czech, y is realized [ɛj]. Does this matter?
Tackling unexpected softness
Infinitive | PRS.1SG | Imperfective | Gloss |
---|---|---|---|
lʲez-tʲ | lʲez-u | -lʲezatʲ | ‘clamber’ |
ɡrɨz-tʲ | ɡrɨz-u | -ɡrɨzatʲ | ‘gnaw’ |
žečʲ | žɡ-u | -žɨɡatʲ | ‘burn’ |
ža-tʲ | žm-u | -žɨmatʲ | ‘press’ |
ža-tʲ | žn-u | -žɨnatʲ | ‘reap’ |
mʲa-tʲ | mn-u | -mʲinatʲ | ‘knead’ |
ras-pʲa-tʲ | ras-pn-u | ras-pʲinatʲ | ‘crucify’ |
What’s the deal with soft consonants before [a]? Three observations:
- Infinitive stems have the shape CVC or CV
- Imperfective stems always have the shape CVC-a
- Present stems are either CVC or CC
- Cʲa occurs in items that CV- in the infinitive and CN- in the present
Rule | mIn-tʲ | mIn-u | mina-tʲ |
---|---|---|---|
Surface Palatalization | mʲIntʲ | mʲInu | mʲinatʲ |
Nasal vowel formation | mʲĩtʲ | ||
Vowel deletion | mʲnu | ||
ĩ → a | mʲatʲ | ||
Softness assimilation | mnu |
- We posit that the root is CVC, with a final nasal
- The vowel is plausibly front
- It triggers Surface Palatalization
- It alternates with a real front vowel in the imperfective
- The back vowel in the infinitive only arises after Surface Palatalization
- Armed with this idea, we can tackle other cases of unexpected softness, even when there are no alternations
- [mʲaso] ‘meat’ ← /minso/
- [lʲubʲitʲ] ‘love.INF’ ← /leubitʲ/
Tackling unexpected hardness
- In Polish, some /e/-initial suffixes trigger Surface Palatalization of non-velars; they usually trigger 1VP or 2VP for the velars
Suffix | Non-velars | Velars |
---|---|---|
LOC.SG | pas-ie [ɕ] ‘belt’ | rzec-e [t͡s] ‘river’ |
V stem | łys-ieć [ɕ] ‘go bald’ | droż-eć [ʐ] ‘become dearer’ |
- When they do not, they do trigger Surface Palatalization of velars
Suffix | Labials | Coronals | Velars |
---|---|---|---|
INS.SG | tłum-em ‘crowd’ | pas-em ‘belt’ | krok-iem [kʲ] ‘step’ |
ADJ.DAT.SG.M | grub-emu ‘fat’ | bos-emu ‘barefoot’ | wielk-iemu [kʲ] ‘big’ |
ADJ.GEN.SG.M | grub-ego | bos-ego | wielk-iego |
In effect, Polish seems to provide ample evidence that just like there is a distinction between ‘softening’ and ‘non-softening’ versions of the high unrounded vowel (traditional i vs. y), there is an entirely parallel ‘softening’ and ‘non-softening’ version of the mid unrounded vowel. This leads us to postulate an underlyingly back /ɤ/ for the latter case (Rubach 1984).
This kind of move is not so well supported by diachrony. Some of the non-palatalizing e’s are indeed historically back vowels that merged with *e — more on Thursday. But most of the time, this case in Polish comes from contraction where the deleted vowel was back: *grubu-jemu > grubēmu. What does this tell us about the diachronic argument in favour of the general approach?
How far can we go?
Our examples so far have been from two kinds of languages:
- Russian or Polish, where [i] and [ɨ] both exist on the surface, and the analysis consists in elaborate reshufflings of the distribution
- Czech or BCMS, where the hard-soft contrast exists as coronal vs. palatal and there is no surface [ɨ]. If we try, we can look for alternative analyses, for example by coalescence with [j]
Morpheme | Labials | Coronals | Velars | UR | |
Czech | INS.SG | dub-em ‘oak’ | hrad-em ‘city’ | rok-em ‘year’ | /-ɛm/ |
LOC.SG | dub-ě [bj] | hrad-ě [ɟ] | roc-e [t͡s] | /-jɛ/ | |
BCMS | ACC.SG | rib-u ‘fish’ | crt-u ‘line’ | drag-u ‘bay’ | /-u/ |
INS.SG | krvlj-u [vʎ] ‘blood’ | smrću [t͡ɕ] ‘death’ | /-ju/ | ||
BCMS | NOM.SG.M | grub-i ‘rough’ | tvrd-i ‘hard’ | jak-i ‘strong’ | /-i/ |
CMP | grublj-i [bʎ] | tvrđ-i [d͡ʑ] | jač-i [t̪͡ʃ̪] | /-ji/ |
Bulgarian plurals
Gender | Number | Labials | Coronals | Velars | UR |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Feminine | SG | riba ‘fish’ | rana ‘wound’ | dъga ‘arc’ | /-a/ |
PL | ribi [b(ʲ?)] | rani [n(ʲ?)] | dъgi [gʲ] | /-ɨ/ | |
Masculine | SG | zъb ‘tooth’ | elen ‘deer’ | vъlk ‘wolf’ | /-∅/ |
PL | zъbi [b(ʲ?)] | eleni [n(ʲ?)] | vъlci [t͡s] | /-i/ |
Bulgarian demonstrates at least two types of [i] suffix.5 Their behaviour is typical in the sense that one does not trigger major place changes of velars, and the other one does (2VP in this case, but there also 1VP triggers). What happens to the other consonants, though?
5 In fact, more than two
Recall that the hard/soft distinction in Bulgarian is neutralized before [i ɛ]: velars are soft in this position (a neutralizing alternation) and the non-velars are ‘semi-soft’, traditionally interpreted as phonemically hard.
Scatton (1975) explicitly appeals to the parallel with Russian in treating the ‘less palatalizing’ [i] as underlying /ɨ/ (with something like Post-Velar Fronting and Surface Palatalization of velars to get the pattern) and the ‘more palatalizing’ [i] as /i/ (with Velar Palatalization rules but nothing happening for other consonants). Bulgarian does not have surface [ɨ] at all!
Further, /ɨ/ and /i/ in this analysis have the exact same effects on preceding consonants. This means we likely have not ‘Post-Velar Fronting’ of /ɨ/ but actually absolute neutralization where /ɨ/ → [i] in all cases. In post-SPE generative phonology, this eventually became extremely problematic (Kiparsky 1968), but these kinds of analysis remained current in generative approaches to Slavic (Gussmann 1980; Rubach 1984; Rubach 1993).
Summary
- The standard analysis of Slavic in early generative phonology assumes highly abstract representations and complicated derivations that largely reproduce diachrony
- The aim is to capture as many generalizations as possible within the phonological component
- The distinction between soft and hard consonants can be derived from the backness of following vowels in underlying representations
- This is seen as economical: storage is expensive, computation is cheap
- Conversely, differences in morphemes’ behaviour with regard to palatalization are encoded in the featural make-up of their segments