Contrast and underspecification
Plan for today
- Underspecification and phonological behaviour
- Issues with lexical contrast and underspecification
- The contrastive hierarchy
Why underspecification?
Contrastive behaviour and underspecification
- We encountered underspecification on Monday to reflect predictable aspects of lexical specification
- We also mentioned ‘linking’ and Structure Preservation as fallbacks for when the phonology tries to do something weird
- We now look at the positive case for underspecification
A simple analysis of final devoicing
Manner | Labial | Coronal | Palatal | Dorsal |
---|---|---|---|---|
Stop | p b | t d | c ɟ | k ɡ |
Affricate | t͡s | t͡ʃ d͡ʒ | ||
Fricative | f v | s z | ʃ ʒ | x ɦ |
Nasal | m | n | ɲ | |
Rhotic | r r̝ | |||
Approximant | l | j |
NOM.SG | GEN.SG | Gloss | NOM.SG | GEN.SG | Gloss |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
xlat | xladu | ‘could’ | mlat | mlata | ‘hammer’ |
ʒlap | ʒlabu | ‘manger’ | xlap | xlapu | ‘man’ |
mraːs | mraːzu | ‘frost’ | ɦlas | ɦlasu | ‘voice’ |
tvaːr̝̥ | tvaːr̝ɛ | ‘cheek’ | lɦaːr̝̥ | lɦaːr̝ɛ | ‘liar’ |
kr̝ɛn | kr̝ɛnu | ‘horseradish’ | dɛn | dnɛ | ‘day’ |
dar | daru | ‘gift’ | t͡sar | t͡sara | ‘czar’ |
- A first attempt:
-
[-syl] → [-voi] / _#
Does this work?
- A better attempt
-
[-syl -son] → [-voi] / _#
Sure, but observe…
- The [-son] segments are exactly the ones that contrast in [±voi]
- As noted earlier, /r/ is the ‘non-nasal non-lateral non-fricative approximant’
- By contrast, ‘stop’ covers both [t] and [d] — we need to specify voicing to narrow it down
A third attempt
Since sonorants are predictably voiced, we are justified in positing a redundancy rule to fill in the [±voi] value. However, unlike the redundancy rules of the lexicon, it must come after the final devoicing rule.
Rule | da[r[ ]voi] | xla[d[+voi]] | xla[p[-voi]] |
---|---|---|---|
+voi → voi / _# | xlat | ||
[ ]voi +son → +voi | da[r[+voi]] |
More evidence for underspecification
Prevocalic | Preconsonantal | Gloss |
---|---|---|
plateb | pla[db]a | ‘payment’ |
hudba | hudeb | ‘music’ |
matek | matka | ‘mother’ |
sladit | sla[tk]ý | ‘sweet’ |
[-syl] → [αvoi] / _[-syl αvoi] … with Structure Preservation
Not so fast…
Prevocalic | Preconsonantal | Gloss |
---|---|---|
bydel | bydlo | ‘livelihood’ |
vyder | vydra | ‘otter’ |
světel | světlo | ‘light’ |
sester | sestra | ‘sister’ |
Sonorants are voiced, but do not trigger voicing assimilation!
This agrees with our findings yesterday that presence/absence of structure corresponds to phonological activity!
A final wrinkle
Rule | /t[v[ ]voi]á[ř[ ]voi]/ | /plat[b[+]voi]a/ | /xla[d[+]voi] |
---|---|---|---|
αvoi → αvoi / _[αvoi] | not applied in /tv/! | pladba | |
[ ]voi → +voi / [v ř] | t[v[+voi]]á[ř[+]voi] | ||
+voi → -voi / _# | tvář̥ | xlat | |
[ ]voi → +voi / [+son] |
Contrastive underspecification
Final devoicing again
- [-syl] → [αvoi] / _[αvoi]
Compare with
- [-son] → [αvoi] / _[-son αvoi]
All the [-son] clause is doing is singling out segment that don’t have a contrastive [±voi] specification. That seems like a hell of a coincidence.
What’s redundant anyway?
Feature | p | b | m |
---|---|---|---|
voi | - | + | |
nas | - | - | + |
What about this?
Feature | p | b | m |
---|---|---|---|
voi | - | + | + |
nas | - | + |
Here, /p/ is the only voiceless phoneme, so it is sufficient to specify it as [-voi]. We no longer need the [-nas]
How do we decide?
Both are fine: this is a point of cross-linguistic variation
The Successive Division Algorithm
For a short(ish) description of the approach, see Dresher (2015); for the full-length treatment, Dresher (2009)
- Take an inventory and assign a + or - value for some feature to every segment in that inventory
- Within each subinventory, repeat the procedure with a different feature
- Once a subinventory consists of one segment, that segment is uniquely specified: stop and do not add any more features to it
- The order of features is not universal
[nas] » [voi]: Czech
[voi] » [nas]
Prediction
Under [voi] » [nas], both voiced obstruents and sonorants have active voicing
- Île de Groix Breton (data from Ternes 1970; analysis by Krämer 2000; Hall 2009)
- unačypaš ‘a crew’ + baːk ‘boat’ → unačypaž baːk
- trizek ‘thirteen’ + miːs ‘month’ → trizeɡ miːs
Fun with the contrastive hierarchy
Cross-linguistic variation: Ifẹ Yoruba
Based on Sandstedt (2018)
Variation in feature ordering → variation in phonological behaviour
ATR | RTR | ||
---|---|---|---|
òɡùrò | ‘spurtle’ | ɔrúkɔ | ‘name’ |
eúrò | ‘bitter-leaf’ | ɛ̀lùbɔ́ | ‘yam flour’ |
oríwo | ‘boil, tumour’ | ɔdídɛ | ‘parrot’ |
èbúté | ‘harbour’ | ɛúrɛ́ | ‘goat’ |
- [+hi] vowels are transparent to ATR harmony
- The hierarchy is [hi] » [ATR]
- [+hi] vowels lack [ATR] specifications and remain inert
Cross-linguistic variation: Standard Yoruba
Ifẹ Yoruba | Standard Yoruba | Gloss |
---|---|---|
ɔrúkɔ | orúkɔ | ‘name’ |
ɛ̀lùbɔ́ | èlùbɔ́ | ‘yam flour’ |
ɔdídɛ | odídɛ | ‘parrot’ |
ɛúrɛ́ | ewúrɛ́ | ‘goat’ |
- Same inventory, but [+hi] vowels initiate a new harmonic span
- [ATR] » [hi]
What have we learned from this?
- ‘Contrast’ is defined at the level of the system
- Not on pairwise comparison
- Not on a priori markedness considerations
- ‘The same’ phonological unit can have different representations in different languages
- The presence of a particular phonetic property (like [+ATR] in Ifẹ Yoruba high vowels) does not guarantee associated phonological behaviour
Underspecification and variation
Persistent underspecification
- We are now considering an architecture where underspecification is not just for the lexicon, but for the phonology too
- How does this relate to phonetics?
Lack of phonological specification is associated with phonetic variability
This is actually a hypothesis developed in the phonetic literature (e.g. Keating 1988a; Keating 1988b), albeit often without an explicit theory of what counts as contrastive.
Languages with no laryngeal contrast
- Hyman (2008), a candidate universal:
All languages have voiceless stops
As a descriptive universal, it is falsified by languages like Yidiɲ that have a single series of stops described as [b d ɟ ɡ]
As an analytical universal, it is a statement about a theoretical object — so what are the stops of Yidiɲ?
Phonetic variation and underspecification
- Kakadelis (2018): three languages with no laryngeal contrast
- Bardi: persistent voicing and manner variation in all stops
- Sierra Norte de Pueblo Nahuatl: variable voicing in all stops, lenition in velars
- Arapaho: no voicing, manner lenition of labials
Conclusion: these languages have the same system of contrast, but different phonetics, so contrast does not matter
An alternative
Based on (Iosad)
On the importance of featural analysis in typology, see Lass (1984); Vaux (2009). For applications of the contrastive hierarchy in typological analysis, see Dresher, Oxford & Harvey (2018); Youssef (2021). For more examples of contrastive hierarchies and synchronic variation, see Natvig (2018); Purnell, Raimy & Salmons (2019)
Contrastive hierarchies and sound change
Contrast shift
- We have seen that the same inventory could be described in terms of different contrastive hierarchies, and thus different patterns of predicted phonological behaviour
Covert reinterpretation of featural specification is a possible type of historical change
Dresher, Harvey & Oxford (2014): ‘contrast shift’
Anglo-Frisian Brightening
- Traditional picture: PGmc /æː/ > PWGmc /aː/ > OE, OFris /æː/
- Motivation
- PGmc /æː/ is uncontroversial
- PWGmc /aː/ is on the basis of back reflexes in OHG, for example
- AF /æː/ is securely attested
- Hogg (1992): the changes are driven by contrast
Height | Front | Back |
---|---|---|
High | iː | uː |
Mid | eː | oː |
Low | æː | aː |
- /æː/ is contrastively front
- WGmc /aː/ merges with /oː/
Height | Front | Back |
---|---|---|
High | iː | uː |
Mid | eː | oː |
Low | æː |
- There is only one low vowel /æː/: frontness is noncontrastive
- This may mean that it has a broader range of phonetic realizations
Height | Front | Back |
---|---|---|
High | iː | uː |
Mid | eː | oː |
Low | æː | ɑː |
- OE, OFris: /ai/ > /aː/ (PGmc stainaz > OE stān)
- The ‘changes’ of /æː/ involve not rules of fronting and backing but the phonetic realization of the long vowel in a changing system of contrast
Formalizing contrast shift
- This is consistent with the Proto-Germanic phonological system:
- /a/-umlaut: lowering of /i u/ to /e o/: change in [±hi]
- Raising of /e/ to /i/ before /i/ (and sometimes /u/): change in [±hi]
Anglo-Frisian
Extension of [±bk] contrast to [+lo] branch (‘cloning’)
Implementing the shift
- Promotion of [±hi] so that it becomes relevant to [+bk] vowels
- Necessarily, this demotes [±lo]
Cloning and new vowels
We can now clone the [+rd] branch to accommodate [-bk +rd] vowels, which phonemicize at this stage
Summary
- Modified Contrastive Specification allows to carefully formalize traditional insights into the role of contrast in diachronic change
I’m not saying anything about the mechanism of this change, or claiming that the contrast system is causing these changes!
Conclusion
Modified Contrastive Specification allows us to make explicit some key insights
- Precise scope of lexical contrast
- Link between presence of structure and phonological activity
- Link between phonological inactivity and phonetic variation
- Diachronic change
Oh and by the way…
The alleged problems for the phoneme identified by Halle can be solved with the contrastive hierarchy (Dresher & Hall 2020).