Markedness and substance
Plan for today
- Contrast and markedness diagnostics
- The contrastive hierarchy, ternary contrasts and unary features
- Building geometrical structure
Contrast and markedness diagnostics
Beyond formal diagnostics
Let’s recall the Big List of Markedness Diagnostics
Marked | Unmarked |
---|---|
less natural | more natural |
more complex | simpler |
more specific | more general |
less common | more common |
unexpected | expected |
not basic | basic |
less stable | stable |
appear in few grammars | appear in more grammars |
later in acquisition | earlier in acquisition |
early loss in language deficit | late loss in language deficit |
implies unmarked feature | implied by marked feature |
harder to articulate | easier to articulate |
perceptually more salient | perceptually less salient |
smaller phonetic space | larger phonetic space |
Which of these do we need to explain?
Default Variability
Inventory | Examples | |
---|---|---|
Stops | /p t/ | Kiowa |
/p k/ | German dialects, Korowai… | |
/t k/ | Nanchang, Badimaya… | |
/p ʔ/ | Jabêm | |
/k ʔ/ | Yaw Burmese | |
Nasals | /m n/ | Trio, Sonora Hiaki… |
/m ŋ/ | Nganasan, Palauan… | |
/n ŋ/ | various Sinitic |
Proposal by Rice (1996)
- Coronals show unmarked behaviour if Coronal is filled in by a default rule
- Not all bare Place nodes in all languages are supplied with Coronal
- Depending on this, either dorsals or coronals are unmarked
- Underspecified dorsals are distinct from highly marked specified dorsals, which are highly marked, potentially within the same language
Predicted behaviours
- Neutralization to velar: delete Place
- Neutralization to coronal: delete Place + insert Coronal
- Coda condition, Selayarese version
- A coda consonant is [ʔ], unless the following consonant is a voiceless stop
- /taʔ-doʔdoʔ/ ‘be sleepy’ \(\rightarrow\) [taʔdoʔdoʔ]
- /taʔ-tuda/ ‘bump against’ \(\rightarrow\) [tattuda]
- Coda has a bare place node but may accept spreading
- Marked dorsals: surface [Dorsal]
‘Placeless’ and ‘real’ dorsals can be phonetically distinct
Yes!
- Japanese
- Spanish (Ramsammy 2013)
Taking stock: what about contrast?
- Rice (2009): lack of phonological contrast → more variation
- At the level of the inventory…
- …or in neutralizing positions
- Lack of contrast arises via markedness reduction → underspecification
- Lack of contrast is compatible with any phonetic realization in principle
- Substantive asymmetries are not phonological
Basically, the reason that neutralization tends to result in glottals or coronals is not that Glottal or Coronal are special features, but that they have properties that are more compatible with being in neutralizing positions.
Ambitiously, other markedness asymmetries could also be not hard-wired but emergent in this way:
- Frequency
- Informativity
- Acquisition
The two jobs of underspecification
So far, underspecification does two jobs:
- Formalization of unmarkedness
- Formalization of contrast
Can we unify the two?
Ternary contrasts and unary features
A ternary contrast: Turkish
‘wing’ | ‘state’ | ‘name’ | |
---|---|---|---|
NOM | kanat | devlet | ad |
PL | kanatlar | devletler | adlar |
ACC | kanadɯ | devleti | adɯ |
- Tripartite behaviour, unpredictable: must be in UR
- Classical analysis: /t/ vs. /d/ vs. /T/
- /T/ → [-voi] word-finally, /T/ → [+voi] otherwise
Ternary contrasts and unary features
- Cases like Turkish are normally taken as a killer argument against unary features: we need [+voi], [-voi] and [0voi]
- The contrastive hierarchy approach has the same issue (Hall 2007)
The entire idea of the contrastive hierarchy is that there is a distinction between
- [+F] (active, marked)
- [-F] (active, marked)
- [0F] (inactive, unmarked)
If the contrastive hierarchy must have binary features, then a markedness difference between [+F] and [-F] can only be stipulated, reversing much of the progress on the link between markedness and size
Geometry to the rescue
Geometry and ternarity
Geometry actually gives us a straightforward way to do more-than-binary contrasts
An example: Breton
Krämer (2000) on Île de Groix Breton:
- Final devoicing
- pout ~ poudew ‘pot’
- kurt ~ kurtew ‘court’
- Turkish-style ternary voicing contrast in word-initial stops
- /p/ fətak paːris → fətak paːris ‘to Paris’
- /b/ unačypaš baːk → unačypaž baːk ‘boat crew’
- /B/ unačypaš bənak → unačypaš pənak ‘any crew’
Reanalysis of Breton
For the gory detail, see Iosad (2017).
- Two-way contrast underlyingly, [voiceless] is marked
- Final devoicing is delinking Lar
Irregular devoicing occurs when a [voiceless]-initial item is preceded by a floating Lar node
- When there is nothing suitable to the left, the floating node docks to the right and expunges [voiceless]
- When there is something suitable to the left, the floating node docks and accepted [voiceless] spreading
- Critically, the same phenomenon occurs in initial mutation
- kozh ‘old’
- ur vamm gozh ‘an old mother’
- ur iliz kozh ‘an old church’
- Floating Lar = trigger of mutation
- Can dock to a preceding delaryngealized obstruent, cannot dock to a preceding sonorant
- Not lexically specific in these cases
Where do bare nodes come from?
We’ve seen bare nodes before, but they were basically stipulated
- Features are privative
- Nodes are assigned to all segments contrastively (un)specific for a feature
- Otherwise we do standard Successive Division
What does this buy us?
- Ternary contrast in a unary framework
- Modified Contrastive Specification insights
- Geometric predictions
It is reasonable to ask whether the bare-nodes framework is just a notational variant of MCS. What does this add?
Well, traditional MCS does not traffic in feature geometry. There is a link from geometric proposals like Node Activation or Default Variability towards the system of contrast in the language, but in ‘pure’ MCS there is no geometry.
Where does structure come from?
Extending the proposal
Sandstedt (2018):
Every split in the contrastive hierarchy introduces a tier
Ifẹ Yoruba yet again
Variation in feature ordering → variation in phonological behaviour
ATR | RTR | ||
---|---|---|---|
òɡùrò | ‘spurtle’ | ɔrúkɔ | ‘name’ |
eúrò | ‘bitter-leaf’ | ɛ̀lùbɔ́ | ‘yam flour’ |
oríwo | ‘boil, tumour’ | ɔdídɛ | ‘parrot’ |
èbúté | ‘harbour’ | ɛúrɛ́ | ‘goat’ |
So what?
The key point that this buys us is that the ordering closed » rtr derives transparency of [closed] vowels with no further stipulation
Cross-linguistic variation: Standard Yoruba
Ifẹ Yoruba | Standard Yoruba | Gloss |
---|---|---|
orúkɔ | ɔrúkɔ | ‘name’ |
èlùbɔ́ | ɛ̀lùbɔ́ | ‘yam flour’ |
odídɛ | ɔdídɛ | ‘parrot’ |
ewúrɛ́ | ɛúrɛ́ | ‘goat’ |
What does this buy us?
- No spreading to [u]: no *[closed, RTR] segment
- This makes [u] a blocker: no spreading to the first syllable under standard autosegmental assumptions (LCC)
- Note that [u] is not an ‘[ATR] harmony trigger’: it has enough structure to block [RTR] spreading but does not seem to trigger anything itself
Structure and contrast
- Structure is created by contrast
- Sandstedt (2018) reports several successful case studies
- However, challenges remain (Nichols 2021; Danesi 2022)
- But note that ‘less structure’ ≠ ‘total absence of structure’!
Markedness, contrast and substance
- Phonological behaviour — including phonological markedness effects — is determined by structure
- Structure comes from contrast
- Substance is useful to implement contrast, but does not define markedness
- Predictions
- Same behaviour, different substance (Dresher 2014)
- The less contrast, the more variation…
- …and the more contrast, the more substantive bias (Rice 2009)