Yer vowels

Plan for today

  • Yer patterns, Havlík and Lower
  • Segmental yer patterns across Slavic
  • Lower, deletion and insertion
  • Yers, palatalization, and mid vowel alternations

Yer patterns

What are yers?

Some examples of vowel-zero alternations
Item Form Ukrainian Polish Slovak BCMS
‘dog’ NOM.SG pes pies pes pas
NOM.PL psɨ psy psy psi
‘dream’ NOM.SG son sen sen san
NOM.PL snɨ sny sny sni
‘coal’ NOM.SG węgiel uhoľ ugao
NOM.PL węgle uhle ugli
‘board’ NOM.SG doška deska doska daska
GEN.PL doščok desek dosák ~ dosiek das(a)ka

Yer patterns: Havlík and Lower

In traditional parlance, yers are either

  • Strong, in which case they merge with some other vowel
  • Weak, in which case they delete

Two main patterns and a minor one

  • Havlík’s Law: weak and strong alternate, starting at the right edge of a sequence
  • Lower Rule: a yer is strong before a yer, weak otherwise
  • Minor pattern: like Lower, but a yer is weak before a voiceless consonant and a weak yer

Yers and morphology

Inflection of n-stem *dьnь ‘day’ in OCS
Case SG PL
NOM dьnь dьne
GEN dьne dьnъ
INS dьnьmь dьnьmi

Havlík

Predicted pattern of alternation under Havlík
Pre-Havlík Havlík Gloss
pьs-ъ pes ‘dog-NOM’
pьs-a psa ‘dog-GEN’
pьs-ьk-ъ psek ‘dog-DIM-NOM’
pьs-ьk-a peska ‘dog-DIM-GEN’
pьs-ьč-ъk-ъ pesček ‘dog-DIM-DIM-NOM’
pьs-ьč-ъk-a psečka ‘dog-DIM-DIM-GEN’
  • Robust in Old Czech, Old Polish, but hardly every found today
    • Cz švec ‘cobbler’, GEN.SG ševce; Ukrainian švec’, GEN.SG ševc’a < *šьvьcь
    • Slk dom ‘house’, dimunitives domok, domček (cf. Cz domeček)
    • Po sejm < sъjьmъ if by levelling from oblique sъjьma etc.

Lower

The Lower pattern in Present-Day Polish
Pre-vocalization Lower Gloss
pьs-ъ pies ‘dog-NOM’
pьs-a psa ‘dog-GEN’
pьs-ьk-ъ piesek ‘dog-DIM-NOM’
pьs-ьk-a pieska ‘dog-DIM-GEN’
pьs-ьč-ъk-ъ pieseczek ‘dog-DIM-DIM-NOM’
pьs-ьč-ъk-a pieseczka ‘dog-DIM-DIM-GEN’

Synchronic corollary

The synchronic consequence is that there can only be one vowel-zero alternation site per paradigm

Segmental patterns of yers

Vocalized yer quality

Language *sъnъ ‘dream’ *dьnь ‘day’ Comment
Ukrainian son den’ ь > e, ъ > o
Russian son d’en’ ь > e + C’, ъ > o
Belarusian son dz’en’ ь > e + C’, ъ > o
Upper Sorbian són dźeń ь > ɛ + C’, ъ > ɔ
Lower Sorbian seń źeń ь > ɛ + C’, ъ > ɛ/a
Polish sen dzień ь > ɛ + C’, ъ > ɛ
Slovak sen den ь > ɛ + C’, ъ > ɛ (but see note)
Czech sen den Almost full merger
Bulgarian sъn den ь > e, ъ > ъ
Macedonian son den ь > e, ъ > o
BCMS san dan Full merger
Slovenian sən dan Full qualitative merger

A typology of yer outcomes

Vowels alternating with zero Difference in consonant behaviour Languages
Multiple Yes Russian, Belarusian, Sorbian: [ɛ ɔ]
Slovak: [ɛ ɔ ɑ ɑː i͡e]
Multiple No Bulgarian: [ъ ɛ]
Macedonian: [ɛ ɔ]
Ukrainian: [ɛ ɔ]
Slovenian: [ə aː]
One Yes Polish: [ɛ] (marginally [ɔ])
One No Czech: [ɛ]
BCMS: [a]

Preliminary summary

What does any theory of yers need to explain?

  • Why do some vowel alternate with zero and others don’t?
  • How do know when to vocalize and when to delete?
  • When the yer vocalizes, what quality does it have?

Previous approaches

The Lower rule

Lightner (1965): the Lower rule for Russian

ĭ ŭ \(\rightarrow\) e o / _ C\(_0\) {ĭ ŭ}, applying left to right

The effect is that all yers before a yer vocalize, but the last yer in a sequence, or a yer before a non-yer vowel, do not and can eventually be deleted

The front yer is a normal front vowel and can do everything that front vowels do:

  • Palatalize preceding consonants
  • Undergo backing once it has merged with /ĕ/

Some Russian derivations

Rule /dĭn+ĭ/ /dĭn+ī/ /dĭn+ĭk+ŭ/ /dĭn+ĭk+ĭk+ŭ/
Palatalization dʲĭnʲĭ dʲĭnʲī dʲĭnʲĭkŭ (dʲĭnʲĭk)ĭkŭ
Lower dʲĕnʲĭ dʲĭnʲī dʲĕnʲĕkŭ (dʲĕnʲĕk)ĭkŭ
Backing dʲĕnʲŏkŭ (dʲenʲŏk)ĭkŭ
Palatalization dʲĕnʲŏčʲĭkŭ
Lower dʲĕnʲŏčʲĕkŭ
Yer deletion dʲĕnʲ dʲnʲī dʲĕnʲŏk dʲĕnʲŏčʲĕk
Late rules dʲenʲ dnʲi dʲenʲok dʲenʲočʲek
Gloss ‘day-NOM’ ‘day-PL’ ‘day-DIM-NOM’ ‘day-DIM-DIM-NOM’

Extending the analysis: Polish

Rule /sOn+O/ /sOn+ɨ/ /dEn+E/ /dEn+i/
Palatalization dʲEnʲE dʲEnʲi
Lower sɛnO sOnɨ dʲɛnʲE dʲnʲi
Yer deletion sɛn snɨ dʲenʲ dʲnʲi
Late rules sɛn snɨ d͡ʑɛɲ dɲi

Further evidence: secondary imperfective ablaut/tensing

Vocalized yer Weak yer Imperfective Gloss
zapiąć [pʲɔɲ] zapnę zapinać ‘fasten’
nadąć [dɔɲ] nadmę nadymać ‘inflate’

Summary of the classical approach

  • Why do some vowel alternate with zero and others don’t?

They are featurally different in the underlying representation

  • How do we know when to vocalize and when to delete?

The Lower rule is sensitive to the features of vowels in the following syllable

  • When the yer vocalizes, what quality does it have?

Determined by the Lower rule

Autosegmentalizing Lower

With the advent of autosegmental phonology, the property of ‘alternating with zero’ could be encoded by means other than segmental features

Autosegmental Lower with defective representations

What does this get us?

  • Any vowel can be a yer: East Slavic, Sorbian, especially Slovak (Rubach 1993), even Polish
  • No special ‘yer subinventory’: yers are featurally regular
  • What is special about yers is prosodic position
  • CVCV phonology: alternation with zero follows from first principles
  • CVCV phonology: clearly articulated link with phonotactics

Phonotactics, deletion, and insertion

Deletion or insertion?

In principle, vowel-zero alternations can be due to either deletion or insertion

  • The standard account relies on deletion
  • Why not insertion? Two reasons
    • Phonotactics
    • Vowel quality

Insertion and phonotactics

  • Insertion could be driven by
    • Avoidance of bad sonority profiles
    • Avoidance of consonant clusters (at word edges) tout court

Yers and cluster avoidance

  • Classic examples aiming to show an absence of general cluster avoidance
    • Russian laska ‘stoat’ ~ lasok ‘GEN.PL’ vs. laska ‘tenderness’ ~ lask ‘GEN.PL’
    • Russian z’erno ‘grain’ ~ z’or’en ‘GEN.PL’ vs. s’erna ‘chamois’ ~ s’ern ‘GEN.PL’
    • Polish trumna ‘coffin’ ~ trumien vs. kolumna ‘column’ ~ kolumn ‘GEN.PL’
    • Slovak octu ‘vinegar.GEN.SG’ ~ ocot ‘NOM.SG’ vs. pocta ‘distinction’ ~ pôct ‘GEN.PL’

Yers and sonority profiles

  • Classic examples showing that suboptimal sonority profiles are tolerated
    • Russian t’eatr ‘theatre’, os’otr ‘sturgeon’
    • Polish wiatr ‘wind’, cyfr ‘figure.GEN.PL’

However…

  • In BCMS1 only coronal fricative-stop clusters are allowed word-finally
    • Everything else is broken up by a vowel, leading to alternations
    • The only vowel involved is [a]
    • vjetar ~ vjetru ‘wind’ like sladak ~ slatki ‘sweet’
    • There is a plausible insertion analysis

Sonority and epenthesis

  • BCMS vjetar ‘wind’, oštar ‘sharp’ ~ vjetri, oštri
  • Bulgarian ogъn ‘fire’, ostъr ‘sharp’ ~ ogn’ove, ostri1
  • Russian v’et’er ‘wind’, ogon’ ‘fire’, v’ód’er ‘bucket.GEN.PL’ ~ v’etrɨ, ogn’i, v’ódra (Isačenko 1970)
    • On the other hand, m’etr ‘metre’

Why not both?

  • Bethin (1992); Scheer (2012) identify a crucial contrast in Polish and Russian
Language UR NOM.SG GEN.SG DIM Gloss
Polish /t͡sɨfr/ cyfra cyfr cyferka ‘figure’
/srebEr/ srebro sreber sreberka ‘silver’
Russian /igl/ igla igl igolka ‘needle’
/kukOl/ kukla kukol kukolka ‘doll’

A prediction

When a vowel is inserted, its quality should be predictable

Yers and predictability: Russian

Is yer quality predictable?

  • Scheer (2011) passim, and many others: no
Context e o
Cʲ_ d’en’ ~ dn’a ‘day’ l’on ~ l’na ‘linen’
C_ * son ~ sna ‘dream’

Halle (1959) referring to Klagstad (1954): yes

Zaliznyak (1967): yes (essentially)

The return of the mid vowel alternation

  • After a hard consonant, the yer is always [o]
  • After a soft consonant, the yer is either [e] or [o]
  • In the classical analysis, this is backwards: the soft consonant is soft because the yer is front
  • The sequence [Cʲo] from /Cĭ/ arises by the sequence of Palatalization > Lower > Backing

An alternative

  • Most notably Farina (1991)
    • Insert [o] after hard consonant
    • Insert [e] after soft consonant, take a ride on the backing rule
  • Scheer (2010b): this would have worked, but the price of the backing rule is underlying /ѣ/ (or too many exceptions)

So, unpredictable after all?

  • Yesterday we developed an account of the e ~ o alternation mostly allowed us to cope with exceptionality
  • Two classes of mid vowel after [Cʲ]
  1. [e] before a softening suffix, [o] elsewhere
  2. Non-alternating [e]
  • It turns out that when a vowel alternates with zero, it is overwhelmingly type 1
  • The exceptions are either conditioned (before j c l’ n’)1 or tiny in number: in the nouns, there is a total of five exceptions (Zaliznyak 1967; Iosad 2020). I can live with that.

Summing up

  • The quality of Russian yers is mostly predictable if
    • We take into account the softness of the preceding consonant
    • We adapt our analysis of mid vowels: when the right context drives the choice, the front outcome is conditioned and the back outcome is the elsewhere
  • We still (mostly) cannot predict when the vowel is inserted or not

Conclusion

Why does this matter?

  • The analysis relies on consonant softness being present before yer quality is resolved: ‘consonant power’
  • This is incompatible with the classical account, where the consonant is soft because the yer is front: ‘vowel power’
  • Who is right?

Consonant power revisited

One final prediction

Scheer (2012): ‘if a vowel is epenthetic, its quality cannot be contrastive’

NOM.SG GEN.PL Derivative Gloss
igla igl igólka ‘needle’
iskra iskr ískorka ‘spark’
nasmork násmoročnɨj ‘cold’
pol’za pol’z pol’éznɨj ‘useful’
vojna vojn vojénnɨj ‘war’
korabl’ korab’él’nɨj ‘ship’
s’el’d’ s’el’ódka ‘herring’
  • The vowels are not yers — but they follow the generalizatons quite precisely
  • The softness of the consonants determines the quality of the vowels, not the other way around

What’s next?

Tomorrow, we reconsider the status of the historically informed traditional approach.

References

Bethin, Christina Y. 1992. Polish syllables: The role of prosody in phonology and morphology. Columbus: Slavica Publishers.
Boyd, Michael Sherman. 1997. Palatalization and coronalization in Russian and Czech: A non-linear approach. Columbus, OH: The Ohio State University PhD thesis.
Farina, Donna Marie. 1991. Palatalization and jers in modern Russian phonology: An underspecification approach. Champaign: University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign PhD thesis.
Halle, Morris. 1959. The sound pattern of Russian: A linguistic and acoustical investigation. ’s Gravenhage: Mouton.
Halle, Morris & Ora Matushansky. 2002. [Αback] assimilation in Russian: An overview. In Aniko Csirmaz, Zhiqiang Li, Andrew Nevins, Olga Vaysman & Michael Wagner (eds.), Phonological answers (and their corresponding questions) (MIT Working Papers in Linguistics 42), 69–80. Cambridge, MA: MITWPL.
Hamilton, William S. 1976. Vowel power versus consonant power in Russian morphophonemics. Russian Linguistics 3(1). 1–18. doi:10.1007/BF00177211.
Hamilton, William S. 1980. Introduction to Russian phonology and word structure. Columbus, OH: Slavica Publishers.
Iosad, Pavel. 2020. Per aspera ad astra: Nuli i zvezdochki v russkoĭ morfonologii. In Andreĭ Aleksandrovich Kibrik, Kseniya Pavlovna Semenova, Dmitriĭ Vladimirovich Sichinava, Sergeĭ Georgievich Tatevosov & Anna Yur’evna Urmanchieva (eds.), VAProsȳ yazȳkoznaniya: Megasbornik nanostateĭ. [A Festschrift for Vladimir Plungian], 69–73. Moscow: Buki Vedi.
Isačenko, Alexander V. 1970. East Slavic morphophonemics and the treatment of the jers in Russian: A revision of Havlík’s Law. International Journal of Slavic linguistics and poetics 13. 73–124.
Klagstad, Jr., Henry L. 1954. Vowel-zero alternations in Modern Standard Russian. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University PhD thesis.
Lightner, Theodore M. 1965. Segmental phonology of Modern Standard Russian. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institue of Technology PhD thesis.
Padgett, Jaye. 2011. Russian consonant–vowel interactions and derivational opacity. In Wayles Brown, Adam Cooper, Alison Fisher, Esra Kesici, Nikola Predolac & Draga Zec (eds.), Formal Approaches to Slavic Linguistics 18: The second Cornell meeting, 2009, 352–381. Ann Arbor, MI: Michigan Slavic Publications.
Rubach, Jerzy. 1993. The lexical phonology of Slovak. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Rubach, Jerzy. 2000. Backness switch in Russian. Phonology 17(1). 39–64. https://www.jstor.org/stable/4420162.
Rubach, Jerzy. 2005. Mid vowel fronting in Ukrainian. Phonology 22(1). 1–36.
Rubach, Jerzy. 2016. Polish yers: Representation and analysis. Journal of Linguistics 52(2). 421–466. doi:10.1017/s0022226716000013.
Scheer, Tobias. 2006. How yers made Lightner, Gussmann, Rubach, Spencer and others invent CVCV. In Piotr Bański, Beata Łukaszewicz & Monica Opalińska (eds.), Studies in constraint-based phonology, 133–207. Warsaw: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Warszawskiego.
Scheer, Tobias. 2010b. Why Russian vowel-zero alternations are not different, and why Lower is correct. Language and Language Behavior 9. 77–112.
Scheer, Tobias. 2010a. A guide to morphosyntax–phonology interface theories: How extra-phonological information is treated in phonology since Trubetzkoy’s grenzsignale. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Scheer, Tobias. 2011. Slavic yers. In Marc van Oostendorp, Colin J. Ewen, Elizabeth Hume & Keren Rice (eds.), The Blackwell companion to phonology. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.
Scheer, Tobias. 2012. Variation is in the lexicon: Yer-based and epenthetic vowel-zero alternations in Polish. In Eugeniusz Cyran, Henryk Kardela & Bogdan Szymanek (eds.), Sound, structure and sense: Studies in memory of Edmund Gussmann, 631–672. Lublin: Wydawnictwo KUL.
Townsend, Charles. 1975. Russian word-formation. Columbus, OH: Slavica Publishers.
Zaliznyak, Andreĭ Anatol’evich. 1967. Russkoe imennoe slovoizmenenie. Moscow: Nauka.